Zur Kurzanzeige

dc.date.accessioned2021-07-29T15:01:41Z
dc.date.available2021-07-29T15:01:41Z
dc.date.issued2016
dc.identifierdoi:10.17170/kobra-202103253602
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/123456789/13050
dc.language.isoengeng
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/*
dc.subjectcolavita effecteng
dc.subjectvisual dominanceeng
dc.subjectauditory dominanceeng
dc.subjectsemantic congruencyeng
dc.subjectperceptioneng
dc.subjectmultisensory processingeng
dc.subject.ddc150
dc.titleSemantic congruency and the (reversed) Colavita effect in children and adultseng
dc.typeAufsatz
dcterms.abstractWhen presented with auditory, visual, or bimodal audiovisual stimuli in a discrimination task, adults tend to ignore the auditory component in bimodal stimuli and respond to the visual component only (i.e., Colavita visual dominance effect). The same is true for older children, whereas young children are dominated by the auditory component of bimodal audiovisual stimuli. This suggests a change of sensory dominance during childhood. The aim of the current study was to investigate, in three experimental conditions, whether children and adults show sensory dominance when presented with complex semantic stimuli and whether this dominance can be modulated by stimulus characteristics such as semantic (in)congruency, frequency of bimodal trials, and color information. Semantic (in)congruency did not affect the magnitude of the auditory dominance effect in 6-year-olds or the visual dominance effect in adults, but it was a modulating factor of the visual dominance in 9-year-olds (Conditions 1 and 2). Furthermore, the absence of color information (Condition 3) did not affect auditory dominance in 6-year-olds and hardly affected visual dominance in adults, whereas the visual dominance in 9-year-olds disappeared. Our results suggest that (a) sensory dominance in children and adults is not restricted to simple lights and sounds, as used in previous research, but can be extended to semantically meaningful stimuli and that (b) sensory dominance is more robust in 6-year-olds and adults than in 9-year-olds, implying a transitional stage around this age.eng
dcterms.accessRightsopen access
dcterms.creatorWille, Claudia
dcterms.creatorEbersbach, Mirjam
dc.relation.doidoi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.015
dc.subject.swdExperimentelle Psychologieger
dc.subject.swdEntwicklungspsychologieger
dc.subject.swdAkustischer Reizger
dc.subject.swdVisueller Reizger
dc.subject.swdWahrnehmungger
dc.type.versionacceptedVersion
dcterms.source.identifiereissn:1096-0457
dcterms.source.journalJournal of Experimental Child Psychologyeng
dcterms.source.pageinfo23-33
dcterms.source.volumeVolume 141
kup.iskupfalse


Dateien zu dieser Ressource

Thumbnail
Thumbnail

Das Dokument erscheint in:

Zur Kurzanzeige

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International
Solange nicht anders angezeigt, wird die Lizenz wie folgt beschrieben: Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International